Rabu, 11 Mei 2016

Analizing Turn-taking Behavior and Gender Differences in A Conversation of Elementary Podcast: A Discourse Analysis Perspective



Analizing Turn-taking Behavior and Gender Differences in A Conversation of Elementary Podcast: A Discourse Analysis Perspective
Denny Nugraha
TBI – B – 6
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
A.  Research Background
The most common kind of spoken language as a matter of fact is conversation, in which one person communicates through speaking to another person or to other people. Just as there are patterns in sounds, words, and sentences, so there are certain patterns in a conversation. Even in the most unpredictable conversations, there are certain devices that people use time and time again. If a person wants to tell a joke or some bad news or to ask for a special favor, there are recognized ways of introducing these subjects. Without these cues, listeners would be very disconcerted. 
Conversations are orderly proceed in an very organized way, but all the participants have to work at the conversation, making sense of things in the world, supporting and reinforcing each other, checking for meaning, and so on. A conversation, unlike a piece of written work of language, is the work of at least two people involved. Thus, this as Talbot (1998) argues, includes the language in use and the language used between them. The language they use help them to do their daily interactional domain and the language used about them describes them and their gender and identity.
When people want to use language to communicate orally with each other, they are faced with a sort of coordination problem. “Avoidance of collision is one obvious ground for this coordination of actions between the participants. In order to communicate efficiently and successfully, they will therefore have to agree to follow certain rules of interaction” (Oreström & Bengt, 1983, p. 18). One such rule is that no one monopolizes the floor but the participants take turns to speak. This important concept in linguistic interaction is called “turn-taking”. 
Turn-taking is made possible by the cognitive limitations of human beings (Beattie, 1982, p. 93). Usually people find it very difficult to talk and to listen simultaneously and therefore there must be some means of allocating turns so that for some limited period one person alone holds the floor and acts primarily as the listener, contributing only briefly to provide support, encouragement, and feedback. In this context the tasks of the present study are firstly to discriminate and secondly to discuss how turn-taking is realized in conversational situations.
Studies of conversation confirm that it is a highly structured activity, and that participants in a conversation follow some conventions in a co-operative way. ‘Taking turn’ at speaking is one of these conventions. But the distribution of talking among speakers is not random; it is definitely governed by ‘turn-taking’ norms and conventions which determine who talks, when, and for how long it takes. A speaker who has not a good knowledge of ‘turn-taking’ rules and techniques is one who does not let others get a word in any conversation.
Conversational conventions such as turn-taking mechanisms, minimal responses, hedges, etc. govern how people take their turns and how many people can talk at once and for how long. They include signals that show ways of letting others speak and – ways of getting the floor. Conversational conventions handle some of this work by providing routines for such things as initiating and ending conversations and for signaling that one is coming to the end of one’s turn – that one is at a “transition-relevant” place, a point where there might be transition to another different speaker.
Gender differences in a conversation have been identified as one fact in connection with turn-taking behavior. This may be a problem that people can experience in their daily conversation. It matters that dominant speakers can often grab too many turns in certain ways of talk. Dr. Brizendine (1994) states that women talk three times as much as men. On the other hand, Drass (1986), in an experiment on gender identity in conversation found that men speak more than women. Therefore, it cannot be instantaneously conclude that dominant speaker comes from one type of gender. There is still a debatable statement and theory about this.
Furthermore, turn-taking and gender differences vary from culture to culture and from language to language. There is no specific rules for ‘taking turns’ for each language but there are general rules for all languages all over the world. One of these general rules is that one must or should listen when another one is speaking, because if this does not happen there will be a kind of miscommunication between the two different speakers.
The emergence of Podcast has been recognized as a source for learning English all over the world. People can simply listen to everyday conversations recorded by native British English speakers in the LearnEnglish Podcasts series, and improve their listening skills. The podcast is hosted by Tess (Female) and Ravi (Male) chatting each other with different people and talking about different everyday topics, such as their favourite celebrities, making plans for the weekend, having a dinner, etc. In line with this, the researcher is interested to explore deeply about gender bias between male and female in taking the turn in a natural conversation. Thus, the researcher arrange this description into the research form under the title “Analizing Turn-taking Behavior and Gender Differences in A Conversation of Elementary Podcast: A Discourse Analysis Perspective”.
B.  Scope of Study
This study will primarily focuses on how gender makes differences in turn-taking behavior especially in a daily conversation. In this study, the researcher set out to explore, in terms of strategy use of turn-taking from the selected conversation produced by two native speaker of british English (male and female). The study will involve both of the audio recording and the dialogue or audioscript of the elementary podcast series 1 episode 1. Differences between male and female in conversation is an important approach in the study of spoken discourse.
C.  Research Questions
1.      How does the turn-taking behavior occur in an English native speakers’ conversation?
2.      How does the gender differences influence the turn-taking behavior?
3.      What are turn-taking strategies or aspects used by different gender of native speakers of English?
4.      What are the impacts of gender differences for the turn-taking activities in the selected conversation?

D.    Aims of The Research
The aims of this research are as follows:
a.       To find out the turn-taking strategies or aspects used by different gender of native speakers of English in the selected conversation.
b.      To explore the impacts of gender differences which may be existed in turn-taking behavior especially in the selected conversation.

CHAPTER II
LITERATURE REVIEW
A.  Turn-taking Behavior
Conversation is distributed on a turn-by-turn basis. A turn constructed by the speaker consists of single words, phrases, clauses or sentences. Stenstrom (1984:11) explains that the chaining principle (cf eg Good 1979), which distinguishes dialogue from monologue, implies conversational partners take turns and the organization of talk refers to turn-taking. A normal string in two-party conversation is A-B-A-B, that is from one speaker turns to the other one (or two-way turn) which can be called the basic structure of talk. According to Stenstrom (1984), the fundamental principles of interaction in terms of turn-taking are accentuated by Schegloff (1972a). Later, according to Zimmerman and West (1975:106-107), Sacks et al (1974) outline a model of turn-taking in naturally occurring conversation which is widely used in the discourse and conversation analysis. The model provides a turn-taking mechanism for conversation and tells us 1) the current speaker may select the next speaker (by asking questions or addressing his or her name directly), then the person selected has the right to speak and meanwhile has to take the turn. 2) If the current speaker does not select the next speaker, any other participants can speak next. 3) If none of above cases happens, the current speaker can hold the floor and continue to speak. According to the model, there are no gap and no overlap between the turns. Sacks et al (1974) say that one party speaks at a time and speaker change recurs (Coates, 2004:112). According to Orestrom (1983:31), Duncan (1972 and 1973) has mentioned four basic types of signals for the operation of the turn-taking mechanism. In general, a proper operation is 1) the current speaker gives a turn-yielding signal, 2) the listener claims the speaker-turn and 3) the current speaker gives up his turn. In a smooth turn-taking process, taking process, the current speaker finishes the speech and the next speaker begins to talk around Transition-Relevant Places (TRP). However, sometimes the listener is uncertain where TRP exactly is; sometimes a lot of people, especially women, speak at the same time when they are excited or enjoying something together; and in some cases, competitive speakers break the rules of turn-taking model on purpose to grab the floor, so simultaneous speech is very common in conversation. These are unsmooth turn-taking processes.
B.     Gender Differences in Conversation
Differences exist in the turn-taking behavior between male and female speakers, which has been claimed by Key (1975) according to Orestrom (1983:146). Female speakers, especially in all female conversations, often break the rule of one person at a time. Many of them speak at the same time. Coates (2004:131) explains that this collaborative mode of organizing talk is first identified as collaborative floor by Carole Edelsky (1993) and calls it a conversational jam session. In this mode, all participants take the conversational floor simultaneously, but the different voices work with each other, not against each other, to construct meaning. Although this way of talking is available to all speakers, Edelsky finds that women are more inclined to use it than men in mixed-sex conversations (Coates, 2004:131). Coates (2004:136) claims that women often adopt the jam session model while men prefer a one-at-atime model of turn-taking, so overlapping talking is rare in all-male talk. Even if men are in a more gladiatorial style of talk, they will also pursue a well-timed exchange of speaker turns. Based on Mary Talbot’s (1992), Coates (2004:137) indicates that men do not like others to join in when they have not finished their speaking. They will think of others’ words as intruding on their right on a solo floor even if the others’ words are collaborative comments and support for their speeches.
With the variation of situation and topic, some things will be changed. Women may obey the rule of one-at-a-time model when they have high social status and are in a very formal conversation. Men may enjoy overlapping each other when they are jointly discussing a topic and become noticeably excited. Differences in turn-taking between males and females do exist.
Men and women pursue different speech behavior in conversation. Coates (2004:126) argues that “it seems that men pursue a style of interaction based on power, while women pursue a style based on solidarity and support.” She also discusses that men pursue competitive behavior while women pursue cooperative behavior. In fact, the gender-differentiated speech behavior in conversation has come into being in childhood. Coates argues that Daniel Maltz and Ruth Borker (1982) claim that girls learn to 1) create and maintain relationships of closeness and equality; 2) criticize others in acceptable ways; and 3) interpret accurately the speech of other girls, while boys learn to 1) assert a position of dominance; 2) attract and maintain an audience; and 3) assert themselves when another speaker has the floor. The girls’ talk is characterized as cooperative and the boys’ as competitive by Maltz and Borker (Coates, 2004:160-161). Among adults, the gender-differentiated speech behaviors continue. Women use more minimal responses, tag-questions, compliments, questions with uncertain rising intonations and mitigating directives with let’s, gonna, can, could, maybe, etc. Women’s speech is softer and more polite. They try to create an environment with closeness and consensus. Men often use explicit and aggressive commands and directives to get the upper hand in conversation to protect themselves psychologically from being pushed around (Tannen, 1992; Coates, 2004).
The workplace is considered as a competitive arena, where men’s competitive behavior and women’s cooperative behavior will be more obvious, since most high positions are occupied by men in the present society. Unfortunately, high status women will be in a dilemma in the workplace. If they use an adversarial, aggressive and competitive speech style, they will be perceived as un-feminine. If they pursue a soft, supportive and cooperative style, they will be considered that they are powerless and do not deserve that high position. Although, men and women pursue very different speech behavior, their common goal of most conversations are to achieve solidarity.
Backchannels are emitted by the listener as feedback signals or minimal responses (Orestrom, 1983:23). They vary considerably in length, from short vocalizations like mm, yeah (which are also called minimal responses) to very long expressions, such as I think you are right. According to Orestrom (1983:107), Duncan & Niederehe (1974) list four types of backchannel and Hene (1978) takes the listener’s role into consideration and gives such items from visual, non-linguistic signs, lexical signs, idioms to sentence completions. Based on the classification of Duncan and Niederehe (1974), Orestrom (1983:107) gives five types of backchannel items: 1) Supports: (mhm, yes, sure, right, OK, fine, I see, that’s nice, that’s right, etc.) the listener has understood the message and expresses acceptance, agreement. 2) Exclamations: (oh, oh dear, oh God, bloody hell, etc.) the listener shows emotional expressions, like surprise. 3) Exclamatory questions: (what, really, did he, was it, etc.) the listener asks short questions with an exclamatory tone. 4) Sentence completions: (a: … eventually, it will come down to more concrete issues…/ b: As she gets more comfortable) the listener completes a sentence that the speaker has begun. 5) Restatements: (a: … having to pick up the pieces/ b: the broken dishes, yeah) the listener briefly restates the speaker’s preceding thought immediately in his or her own words. As these examples show, Stenstrom (1994:81) argues that backchannels can reflect empathy, enthusiasm and indignation. Moreover she makes a further argument that they can also reflect a lack of interest, indifference and impatience sometimes.  
Backchannels, as a signal of feedback, are preferred by women, but some types of backchannels, such as mhm and yeah, are used by men much more than women. People give backchannels with three tones—falling tone, level tone and rising tone. The finding in Stenstrom’s (1994:121) study is that the falling tone is the most common one, followed by the level tone. She also finds that men use level tone three times more than women.
Therefore, conversation is based on give and take. Turns can flow naturally and smoothly. There are rules that organize our talk and without them misunderstanding may occur. Take, for example, if two persons speak at the same time, they will not understand each other until one of them listens to hear the other. As it has been mentioned in chapter two that turn-taking is a fundamental organizational feature of conversation. It is a complex technique and hard to distinguish in an informal discourse. It depends on power and status, who takes the first turn, who interrupts more and who falls silent. An interruption occurs when the interrupter does not know when to enter a conversation: i.e. at ‘Transition Relevance Places’ (TRPs). In brief, to analyze the gender differences with the turn-taking behavior in a conversation, it is useful to review the aspects of turn-taking itself.


CHAPTER III
DATA AND ANALYSIS
A.      Data
The data of this research involves the audio files and also with the transcript of the conversation of podcast episode 1 series 1, episode 1 series 2, episode 1 series 3, and episode 1 series 4 respectively in order to see the differences of two speakers. In addition, the researcher only take one section in each episode of the series provided. The data was downloaded primarily from the website https://learnenglish.britishcouncil.org/en/elementary-podcasts. The conversation is definitely an interactional type in which speakers involved are engaged to take turns without any intense to get things done like in transactional one. Here are the sample of transcript of the conversation in each series:
Episode 1 series 1
Section 1 - Susan, this is Paul – introducing your friends
Ravi     : Hello, and welcome to LearnEnglish elementary podcast number one. My name’s Ravi…
Tess     : … and I’m Tess. We’re your presenters and we’ve got lots of things for you to listen to today, but before we start, I think we should introduce ourselves. Ravi?
Ravi     : OK … erm … I’m Ravi.
Tess     : Or, I tell you what, I’ll introduce you and you can introduce me. How about that?
Ravi     : Well, OK then. Erm, this is Tess. She’s from London. She’s … how old are you?
Tess     : None of your business, Ravi!
Ravi     : And she loves dancing and riding her mountain bike. OK?
Tess     : OK. And this is Ravi. He comes from Manchester. He’s 23. Oh … aren’t you?
Ravi     : Oh yes.
Tess     : He likes football, and … he’s a great cook.
Ravi     : Thanks!
                                                                                                                                               
Episode 1 series 2
Section 1 - It's Tess's birthday today…!
Ravi    : Hello and welcome to the Second series of the LearnEnglish Elementary podcast. This is podcast number one. If you listened to the first series you’ll remember – I hope – that my name’s Ravi…
Tess     : … and I’m Tess. Yes, we’re back again. I hope you didn’t miss us too much. We’ve had a little break – did you go anywhere nice Ravi?
Ravi     : No.
Tess     : – and now we’re back with more good stuff for you to listen to. We’ve got…
Ravi    : Aren’t you going to tell them, Tess? I’ll tell them if you don’t. We’ve chosen a special day to come back – it’s Tess’s birthday today, isn’t it?
Tess     : Yeah, it is.
Ravi     : Happy birthday!
Tess     : Thank you.
Ravi     : Are you doing anything special?
Tess     : Well, I’m going out for dinner with some friends tonight and then we might go to a club. I’m not really sure.
Ravi     : Sounds good. Where are you going to eat? Is it somewhere a bit special or …
Tess     : Yeah, it’s a French place. I’ve heard it’s really good but I haven’t been there.
Ravi     : I’m sure it’ll be great. Any good presents?
Tess     : Well, I got some money from my mum and dad to buy myself something nice so I’m going to get some new boots with that. And that’s it, so far ….
Ravi     : Well, I’ll get you a coffee when we finish here, OK?
Tess     : OK. And a cake?
Ravi     : Oh go on then. A small one. But let’s get on with it. What have we got today?
Tess     : We’ve got our quiz, then we’ve got people telling us about their favourite food. And Carolina’s back again.
Ravi     : Right. If you’re listening and you don’t remember Carolina, she’s a student from Venezuela who’s come to Britain to study and we follow her in every podcast to hear how she’s getting on.
                                                                                                                                               
Episode 1 series 3
Section 1 – Welcome Everyone
Ravi    : Hi everyone, I’m Ravi.
Tess     : And I’m Tess.
Ravi    : And our job is to tell you about some of the things you think are typically British. In this series we’ll tell you about British weather, the royal family, British pets… lots of ‘typical’ British things. But today we’re going to tell you about a very famous British food – a dish that lots of people think is the typical British thing to eat. Do you know what I’m talking about? It’s fish and chips.
Tess     : Mmm, nice. It’s true that lots of people think that fish and chips is what British people eat all the time. My Spanish friends really wanted to try fish and chips when they came to visit. So, OK then Ravi – tell us what you’ve learnt.
Ravi    : Well, first of all, I’ll say a little bit about what fish and chips are – people don’t always know. It’s a piece of fish, covered in batter and deep fried..
Tess     : Can you tell us what batter is?
Ravi    : It’s a mixture of flour and water, usually. You cover the fish...
Tess     : What kind of fish is it?
Ravi    : Good question. It can be different kinds of fish but the most popular are cod and haddock – two fish that were very common in the sea around Britain.
Tess     : Are they not so common now?
Ravi    : No, they aren’t – but I’ll talk about that in a minute. So, you cover the fish in batter and then you deep fry it – that means you cook it in a deep pan of very hot oil, vegetable oil usually. You cook the chips the same way, you deep fry the potatoes in very hot oil. Tess: And chips are chips, aren’t they? Americans call them ‘fries’ or ‘French fries’ but we call them chips.
                                                                                                                                               
Episode 1 series 4
Section 1 – Welcome Back
Tess     : Hello again, everyone. My name's Tess.
Ravi    : And I'm Ravi.
Tess     : And, as usual, we're here to talk about Britain. The things that you know about it …
Ravi    : … and the things you think you know. We've already talked about a lot of different topics – things that you think are typically British. And today we've got something nice. Tess, how's Oscar?
Tess     : He's fine, thanks. Why?
Ravi    : I'm introducing the topic, Tess. Pets.
Tess     : Ahh. Very clever. Ravi: Oscar is Tess's cat.
Tess     : And a member of the family.
Ravi    : Hmm ... A lot of people believe that the British are crazy about animals. We're often called 'a nation of pet lovers'. But is it true? To begin with – do we have more pets than anyone else? So I did a bit of research. And yes, a lot of people in Britain have pets – 49 per cent of British homes – that's nearly half – have an animal.
Tess     : Is that a lot?
Ravi    : That's nearly 30 million pets – cats, dogs, rabbits, hamsters, parrots – but not including fish! That's a lot of pets. But there are a lot of other places with as many pets as Britain, or even more ... the USA, New Zealand, some countries in Europe – 62 million homes in Europe have pets. Britain isn't that unusual.
Tess     : And it depends what you count as a pet.
Ravi    : Well, yes, that's true.
Tess     : Cats and dogs are the most common pets in Britain, right?
Ravi    : Well, actually, they now think that the most popular pets in Britain are ...?
Tess     : Well, if it isn't dogs or cats … um … I don't know. Rabbits?
Ravi    : Nope ... Give up? ... Reptiles. Snakes and lizards and iguanas and things. And insects – spiders are really popular now.
Tess     : Yeuk. I'm not sure I could love a spider.
Ravi    : Nor me.  Tess: Do you want to know something interesting?
Ravi    : Erm, yes.
Tess    : Well, Britain was the first country to have an organisation to look after animals. The RSPCA. That's the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals. It was started in the nineteenth century when things were bad for animals in Britain – for horses, dogs, all of them. So some people started the RSPCA. But – and this is the important thing – there were no organisations or charities at that time to protect children or babies or people at all. And life was really hard in the nineteenth century.
Ravi    : Wow.
Tess     : So people started to say that the British care about animals more than people.
Ravi    : But don't you sometimes think it's true? That the British like animals more than people? We give more money to animal charities, like the RSPCA, than to charities for people. That's a fact. And there are lots of programmes about animals on television and stories in the newspapers.
Tess     : I know. It's true. And do you know, people who ask for money on the street, well, if they have a dog with them, then they get more money.
Ravi    : Ha! Is that true? Money for the dog, but not for the person?
Tess     : Yep.
Ravi    : So we are crazy about animals. It's true. You'll have to go home and tell Oscar.
Tess     : I think he already knows.
                                                                                                                                                            



The data above will be analyzed in the following subchapter.
3.1  Data Analysis
There are different methods to be used for analyzing data. In fact, the choice between different research methods should depend on what the researcher attempts to find out. As Silverman (2001:25) quoted by Abdelrahim (2006) argues, if one wants to discover how people tend to vote, then a qualitative method seem to be the most appropriate choice. On the other hand, if one is concerned with exploring people’s life histories, or everyday behaviour, then qualitative methods may be favoured. He also points out, that the audio-taped data is qualitative. There are four major methods used by qualitative researchers: observation, analyzing texts and documents, interviews and recording and transcribing. These methods, as Silverman (2001) says, can be used both in quantitative and qualitative research. Transcripts of audio or video recordings are rarely used in quantitative research, because of the assumption that they are difficult to quantify.
As mentioned before, the researcher used transcripts of audio recordings to collect the data. These audio recordings were transcribed and then analyzed to look at some conversational tactics between two English native speakers, male and female. Each of these tactics was analyzed separately. These tactics are ‘topic and topic development’, ‘minimal responses’ and ‘turn-taking mechanisms’ which include ‘interruptions’ and ‘overlaps’.
Below is a description of the steps the researcher followed in the process of analyzing the differences between males and females of the native speakers in the conversations.
a.       The researcher transcribed the recordings, so that he could focus directly on these gender differences.
b.      The researcher started with how topics were handled between ‘male’/’female’? How did they start, choose, develop and discuss their topics? What kinds of conversational devices such as ‘question-asking’ did they use?
c.       The number of ‘minimal/back-channel responses’, interruptions and overlaps used by the subjects, were counted and tabulated. The researcher used Zimmerman and West’s (1975) table which counted the frequencies of the irregularities of ‘turn-taking’ in conversation.
d.      The analysis of the data led to the results of the study, which were the frequencies of conversational ‘turn-taking’ differences between male and female speakers. Thus, the researcher used techniques of counting and tabulation to analyze the data.

3.1.1        Topic development and back-channel responses
The two native speakers, male and female, are in the same age and work in the British Council foundation. They were young and were colleagues. Contrastively, they come from different city in Britain, that is Ravi comes from Manchester and Tess comes from London. Both of them seem to be confident and friendly in every episodes of series of Podcast as they are the reporters of the program.
3.2.1.1 Episode 1 series 1 analysis
The table 1 in the following describes the analysis of the turn-taking in the episode 1 series 1:
Table 1
Turn
Person/sayer
Dialogue
Aspects
Topic Development
Back-channel Response
Topic starter
Topic changer
Question-asking
1
Ravi
Hello, and welcome to LearnEnglish elementary podcast number one. My name’s Ravi…
-
-
-
-

Tess
… and I’m Tess. We’re your presenters and we’ve got lots of things for you to listen to today, but before we start, I think we should introduce ourselves. Ravi?
Yes
-
Yes
-
2
Ravi
OK … erm … I’m Ravi.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)

Tess
Or, I tell you what, I’ll introduce you and you can introduce me. How about that?
-
-
Yes
-
3
Ravi
Well, OK then. Erm, this is Tess. She’s from London. She’s … how old are you?
-
-
Yes
Yes (followed by full response)

Tess
None of your business, Ravi!
-
-
-
-
4
Ravi
And she loves dancing and riding her mountain bike. OK?
-
-
Yes
-

Tess
OK. And this is Ravi. He comes from Manchester. He’s 23. Oh … aren’t you?
-
-
Yes
Yes (not followed by full response)
5
Ravi
Oh yes.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)

Tess
He likes football, and … he’s a great cook.
-
-
-
-
6
Ravi
Thanks!
-
-
-
-
Total
1
0
5
4

As described in the table 1, in the first episode of the first series, the topic is about the introduction of both of them as the presenters of the program. From this conversation, Tess has initiated that they should introduce theirselves. Ravi respond to Tess’s signal (question to Ravi), but he starts with a confirmation, following by a backchannel response ‘erm’ and then says his name. Tess correspondingly respond Ravi’s introduction with telling him the other way of introducing theirselves that is by introducing each other.
There is an interesting thing in this episode that Tess uses more questions that need confirmation from Ravi or they can be called as question tags. The first question from Tess is as just explained above that she signals Ravi to introduce himself. Then, the second one is about the confirmation to persuade Ravi about her idea of the introduction. And the last one is her uncertainty of Ravi’s age. Furthermore, Ravi only uses two question tags based on the topic of introduction. The first is to confirm about Tess’s age but then she does not answer the correct response to it. Then, the last is when he finished mentioning Tess’s hobbies which are dancing and riding her mountain bike.
3.2.1.2 Episode 1 series 2 analysis
Then, the table 2 in the following describes the analysis of the turn-taking in the episode 1 series 2 as discussed:
Table 2
Turn
Person/sayer
Dialogue
Aspects
Topic Development
Back-channel Response
Topic starter
Topic changer
Question-asking
1
Ravi
Hello and welcome to the Second series of the LearnEnglish Elementary podcast. This is podcast number one. If you listened to the first series you’ll remember – I hope – that my name’s Ravi…
-
-
-
-

Tess
… and I’m Tess. Yes, we’re back again. I hope you didn’t miss us too much. We’ve had a little break – did you go anywhere nice Ravi?
Yes
-
Yes
-
2
Ravi
No.
-
-
-
-

Tess
– and now we’re back with more good stuff for you to listen to. We’ve got…
-
-
-
-
3
Ravi
Aren’t you going to tell them, Tess? I’ll tell them if you don’t. We’ve chosen a special day to come back – it’s Tess’s birthday today, isn’t it?
-
Yes
Yes
-

Tess
Yeah, it is.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)
4
Ravi
Happy birthday!
-
-
-
-

Tess
Thank you.
-
-
-
-
5
Ravi
Are you doing anything special?
-
-
-
-

Tess
Well, I’m going out for dinner with some friends tonight and then we might go to a club. I’m not really sure.
-
-
-
-
6
Ravi
Sounds good. Where are you going to eat? Is it somewhere a bit special or …
-
-
Yes
-

Tess
Yeah, it’s a French place. I’ve heard it’s really good but I haven’t been there.
-
-
-
Yes (followed by full response)
7
Ravi
I’m sure it’ll be great. Any good presents?
-
-
Yes
-

Tess
Well, I got some money from my mum and dad to buy myself something nice so I’m going to get some new boots with that. And that’s it, so far ….
-
-
-
-
8
Ravi
Well, I’ll get you a coffee when we finish here, OK?
-
-
Yes
-

Tess
OK. And a cake?
-
-
Yes
-
9
Ravi
Oh go on then. A small one. But let’s get on with it. What have we got today?
-
-
Yes
-

Tess
We’ve got our quiz, then we’ve got people telling us about their favourite food. And Carolina’s back again.
-
-
-
-
Total
1
1
7
2

As depicted in the table 2, in the episode one of the second series, the topic is about Tess’s birthday. As usual, they welcome the listeners and recall about this program back in mind. The topic was presented by Ravi as he ask Tess whether she will tell the listeners or not. According to Ravi, he said that they have chosen that day as the special day to come back. But, Ravi uses question tag to confirm about that.
Furthermore, in this episode, Tess uses minimal response or back-channel response yeah as the confirmation of Ravi’s tag. The topic then moves to the upcoming events that will be done by Tess in her birthday at that moment. The movement of the topic is initiated by Ravi as Tess respond to his expression. Not like the first episode in the first series, in this series, there is a clear movement of the topic in the conversation.
3.2.1.3 Episode 1 series 3 analysis
Then, the table 3 in the following describes the analysis of the turn-taking in the episode 1 series 3 as discussed above:
Table 3
Turn
Person/sayer
Dialogue
Aspects
Topic Development
Back-channel Response
Topic starter
Topic changer
Question-asking
1
Ravi
Hi everyone, I’m Ravi.
-
-
-
-

Tess
And I’m Tess.
-
-
-
-
2
Ravi
And our job is to tell you about some of the things you think are typically British. In this series we’ll tell you about British weather, the royal family, British pets… lots of ‘typical’ British things. But today we’re going to tell you about a very famous British food – a dish that lots of people think is the typical British thing to eat. Do you know what I’m talking about? It’s fish and chips.
Yes
-
Yes
-

Tess
Mmm, nice. It’s true that lots of people think that fish and chips is what British people eat all the time. My Spanish friends really wanted to try fish and chips when they came to visit. So, OK then Ravi – tell us what you’ve learnt.
-
-
-
Yes (followed by full response)
3
Ravi
Well, first of all, I’ll say a little bit about what fish and chips are – people don’t always know. It’s a piece of fish, covered in batter and deep fried..
-
Yes
-
-

Tess
Can you tell us what batter is?
-
-
Yes
-
4
Ravi
It’s a mixture of flour and water, usually. You cover the fish...
-
-
-
-

Tess
What kind of fish is it?
-
Yes
Yes
-
5
Ravi
Good question. It can be different kinds of fish but the most popular are cod and haddock – two fish that were very common in the sea around Britain.
-
-
-
-

Tess
Are they not so common now?
-
-
Yes
-
6
Ravi
No, they aren’t – but I’ll talk about that in a minute. So, you cover the fish in batter and then you deep fry it – that means you cook it in a deep pan of very hot oil, vegetable oil usually. You cook the chips the same way, you deep fry the potatoes in very hot oil.
-
-
-
-

Tess
And chips are chips, aren’t they? Americans call them ‘fries’ or ‘French fries’ but we call them chips.
-
-
Yes
-
Total
1
2
5
1

Next, as analyzed in the table 3, in the first episode of the third series, the topic is mainly about what things are included to typically British. The first thing of this topic is introduced by Ravi that is about fish and chips. Based on what has said by Tess, fish and chips is what British people eat all the time. Then, Ravi moves the first topic to the next topic that is about the terms of batter and deep fry. Subsequently, Tess changes the topic with her question to Ravi that is about the kind of fish. It is rather back to the first topic about fish and chips.
Moreover, in this episode, Tess uses minimal response or what is called back-channel response Mmm as she starts to give her opinion about what has been said by Ravi. But the movement of the topics in this episode is centered to Tess who mostly changes the topic that is initiated by Ravi in the first line of the conversation. Therefore, as like in the second series, Tess uses again the back-channel response more than Ravi does.
3.2.1.4 Episode 1 series 4 analysis
Then, the table 4 in the following describes the analysis of the turn-taking in the episode 1 series 4:
Table 4
Turn
Person/sayer
Dialogue
Aspects
Topic Development
Back-channel Response
Topic starter
Topic changer
Question-asking
1
Tess
Hello again, everyone. My name's Tess.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
And I'm Ravi.
-
-
-
-
2
Tess
And, as usual, we're here to talk about Britain. The things that you know about it …
-
-
-
-

Ravi
… and the things you think you know. We've already talked about a lot of different topics – things that you think are typically British. And today we've got something nice. Tess, how's Oscar?
Yes
-
Yes
-
3
Tess
He's fine, thanks. Why?
-
-
Yes
-

Ravi
I'm introducing the topic, Tess. Pets.
-
-
-
-
4
Tess
Ahh. Very clever.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)

Ravi
Oscar is Tess's cat.
-
-
-
-
5
Tess
And a member of the family.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
Hmm ... A lot of people believe that the British are crazy about animals. We're often called 'a nation of pet lovers'. But is it true? To begin with – do we have more pets than anyone else? So I did a bit of research. And yes, a lot of people in Britain have pets – 49 per cent of British homes – that's nearly half – have an animal.
-
-
-
Yes (followed by full response)
6
Tess
Is that a lot?
-
-
Yes
-

Ravi
That's nearly 30 million pets – cats, dogs, rabbits, hamsters, parrots – but not including fish! That's a lot of pets. But there are a lot of other places with as many pets as Britain, or even more ... the USA, New Zealand, some countries in Europe – 62 million homes in Europe have pets. Britain isn't that unusual.
-
-
-
-
7
Tess
And it depends what you count as a pet.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
Well, yes, that's true.
-
-
-
-
8
Tess
Cats and dogs are the most common pets in Britain, right?
-
Yes
Yes
-

Ravi
Well, actually, they now think that the most popular pets in Britain are ...?
-
-
Yes
-
9
Tess
Well, if it isn't dogs or cats … um … I don't know. Rabbits?
-
-
Yes
Yes (followed by full response)

Ravi
Nope ... Give up? ... Reptiles. Snakes and lizards and iguanas and things. And insects – spiders are really popular now.
-
-
-
-
10
Tess
Yeuk. I'm not sure I could love a spider.
-
-
-
Yes (followed by full response)

Ravi
Nor me.
-
-
-
-
11
Tess
Do you want to know something interesting?
-
Yes
Yes
-

Ravi
Erm, yes.
-
-
-
Yes (followed by full response)
12
Tess
Well, Britain was the first country to have an organisation to look after animals. The RSPCA. That's the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals. It was started in the nineteenth century when things were bad for animals in Britain – for horses, dogs, all of them. So some people started the RSPCA. But – and this is the important thing – there were no organisations or charities at that time to protect children or babies or people at all. And life was really hard in the nineteenth century.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
Wow.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)
13
Tess
So people started to say that the British care about animals more than people.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
But don't you sometimes think it's true? That the British like animals more than people? We give more money to animal charities, like the RSPCA, than to charities for people. That's a fact. And there are lots of programmes about animals on television and stories in the newspapers.
-
Yes
Yes
-
14
Tess
I know. It's true. And do you know, people who ask for money on the street, well, if they have a dog with them, then they get more money.
-
-
-
-

Ravi
Ha! Is that true? Money for the dog, but not for the person?
-
-
Yes
-
15
Tess
Yep.
-
-
-
Yes (not followed by full response)

Ravi
So we are crazy about animals. It's true. You'll have to go home and tell Oscar.
-
Yes
-
-
16
Tess
I think he already knows.
-
-
-
-
Total
1
4
9
7

Lastly, as tabulated in the table 4, in the first episode of the fourth series, Tess acts the first one who starts the conversation of the program. Ravi opens for the first topic as he ask Tess about her cat named Oscar. Ravi then introduces the topic together with his answer to Tess’s question of why he wants to know about her cat. Thus, the topic is about pets that are such cat (mentioned to Oscar), dogs, rabbits, hamsters etc.
The movement of the topic is initiated firstly by Tess that she ask Ravi whether he wants to know something interesting or not. The interesting thing is that British people care about animals or pets more than people. This topic then is supported by the statement people give more money to animal charities, like the RSPCA (Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals), than to charities for people. According to Tess, that is a fact. And there are lots of programs about animals on television and stories in the newspapers.
Then, in this episode, Tess also uses more minimal responses such as ahh, um, yeuk, and yep than Ravi. Whereas, Ravi uses less minimal responses than Tess do such as hmm and erm. Moreover, Tess uses more question tags as the confirmation of Ravi’s statement or question such as Why?, and about her uncertainty right? And Rabbits? Then, Ravi’s question tag is to show uncertainty such as are ...?
3.1.2        Interruptions and overlaps
Minimal response or back-channel response as has been discussed above, such as yeah or mhm do not constitute a turn; people are a way of showing that the speaker has got the listener’s attention and interest. Zimmerman and West (1975), argue that there are two irregularities or violations of normal turn-taking in conversation. They are interruptions and overlaps which after them speakers tend to fall silent than other speaker. According to data, average silence in the conversations lasted for 1.35 seconds. The listener has an active role in the conversation, not a passive one. If the listener does not use these minimal responses, the speaker may seem as if he is talking to himself.
Then, according to the data of recordings and audioscripts, the researcher could not find any kinds of interruptions and overlaps. It is mostly because the program is the elementary products that refer to the listeners who are still in the beginning to intermediate level of learning English. This can make them easy to understand and recognize the topic and the vocabularies which are used in the conversation by both speakers. Thus, interruptions and overlaps definitely will interfere the listeners to catch the ideas or topics from the speakers.
Based on the above analysis of the topic development and minimal responses, there are several differences occurred in the conversation between Ravi and Tess. The table 5 below depicts and tabulates the results of those differences as analyzed above:
Table 5
Aspects
Gender
Male Speaker
Female Speaker
Topic development
·      Topic starter: 3 of 4 starts
·      Topic changer: 4 of 7 changes
·      Question-asking: 12 of 26 questions
·      Topic starter: 1 of 4 starts
·      Topic changer: 3 of 7 changes
·      Question-asking: 14 of 26 questions
Back-channel responses (followed or not followed by the full response)
·      Back-channel responses: 6 of 14 responses
·      Back-channel responses: 8 of 14 responses

3.2  Summary of The Analysis
Four audio-recordings of the first section of the first episode from four series have been transcribed and analyzed to see how far gender affects conversation especially in turn-taking activities. Both female and male native speaker of English in the program are involved in this analysis. Then, the analysis has arrived to the following results:
1.      Female speaker uses ‘question-asking’ a lot in her conversation which shows that she faced some troubles in the topic and she failed in developing and discussing it.
2.      Unlike female speaker, male speaker succeeded in choosing and developing their topics. This supports the claims that men are ‘decision-makers’ and that they have power of decision making more than women.
3.      Male speaker, based on the analysis, starts the conversation more than female in the selected program. Male speaker also changes the topic very often than female.
4.      Female speaker uses minimal responses or back-channel responses more than male speaker in the conversation selected.

CHAPTER IV
CONCLUSION
4.1         Conclusion
The present study aimed to investigate gender differences and interactional features of turn-taking activities in an English learning program. The conventions involved are ‘topic and topic development’, ‘minimal/back-channel responses’ and ‘turn-taking mechanisms’ which also include ‘interruptions’ and ‘overlaps’. To achieve this objective the interactional conversations were analyzed and discussed, and the analysis has shown important findings. That is, interactional conventions vary between male and female speaker. These differences were analyzed and discussed in details in chapter three. The subjects who have participated in the experiments were two English native speakers.
In conclusion, the turn-taking activities occur variously by some features and mechanism of conversation. Those features or strategies as mentioned above which consciously or unconsciously are conducted such as topic development consists of three different aspects; topic changer, topic starter, and question-asking; and then also the consideration about back-channel responses used by both speakers. Based on the analysis, turn-taking happens as one of the speakers starts each conversation. Indeed, the turn-taking mechanism or strategies mostly influence these features in advance.
According to the dialogues between male and female speaker of English, it can be concluded that indeed gender plays an important role in turn-taking. In advance, male speaker is known as he has more advantages behavior in some respects than female speaker. As described in the last chapter that male speaker succeeded in choosing and developing their topics. This supports the claims that men are ‘decision-makers’ and that they have power of decision making more than women. Then, male speaker, based on the analysis, starts the conversation more than female in the selected program. Male speaker also changes the topic very often than female. But instead female speaker uses minimal responses or back-channel responses more than male speaker in the conversations selected. Therefore, the researcher overall concludes that male speaker is more dominant than female speaker especially in four selected dialogues of selected program.


4.2         Recommendations
The researcher believes that the following recommendations may prove to be useful for further research:
1)      Due to the big significance of conversation as our daily life interaction, an intensive work and studies should be done on this area.
2)      The researcher recommends the further research or analysis on women’s language attitude to be compared with men’s language attitude.
3)      Further studies should be conducted on a larger sample on areas of conversation in order to include large groups of participants and individual differences.

References:
LearnEnglish elementary podcast series or No. 1 – 4 (audio files and audioscripts), downloaded on https://learnenglish.britishcouncil.org/en/elementary-podcasts  at 18th of May 2016
McCarthy, Michael. 2000. Discourse Analysis for Language Teachers. New York: Cambridge University Press
Abdelrahim, Naglaa Mohammed. 2006. Turn-taking Behavior and Gender Differences in Speech: A Case Study of A Group of Educated Sudanese. University of Khartoum: Unpublished thesis
Wei-dong, Yang. 2007. Realizations of Turn-taking in Conversational interactions. US-China Foreign Language, ISSN1539-8080, USA, Volume 5, No.8 (Serial No.47)
Ghilzai, Shazia Akbar. 2015. Conversational Analysis of Turn taking Behavior and Gender Differences in Multimodal Conversation. Unpublished Journal